Successfully recognizing and avoiding predators can have immense fitness consequences [1], but individual variation in anti-predator behaviour remains poorly understood. One well-studied factor is learning to identify predators, which is important to effectively focus anti-predator behaviour on potentially novel threats and to decrease costs of wasted defensive behaviours [2, 3]. Learning can occur at an individual level, providing direct and accurate information, but increasing risk for the observer due to the proximity to the threat. Learning can alternatively occur at a social level, where the sources of information are conspecifics and their responses to the threat. Such social learning reduces the risk to the observer, but also provides potentially less accurate information [4]. Differences in the recognition of and response to predators are further amplified by individual variations in learning accuracy and personality [5, 6]. Considering the evolutionary importance of predation-avoidance, such individual differences may have considerable fitness impacts [2].
A less-studied contributor to individual variation in anti-predator behaviour are social dynamics. Social factors such as sex or dominance might heavily influence individual motivation to participate in anti-predator behaviour [7]. A better understanding of the importance of social dynamics on motivational variation is interesting in its own right, and would also allow better control for motivation when studying variation in learning accuracy. Studies on predator learning to date were either conducted at an individual or a group level (e.g. [8,9,10,11,12]). In the absence of social partners, individual testing may provide similar levels of experienced threat, and therefore similar motivation to engage in anti-predator behaviour, for all subjects. On the other hand, group testing can examine social dynamics and their impact on motivational levels, but they cannot distinguish between whether an individual has failed to learn to recognise a predator, or is simply unmotivated to respond to it. Studies conducted in the wild also face additional difficulties in recognizing individual study subjects (e.g. [13]; but see [14, 15]). Only by combining both group and individual paradigms for the same identifiable individuals can we tease out the specific role of social dynamics on engagement in anti-predator behaviour.
We examined an important anti-predator behaviour—alarm calling—in common ravens (Corvus corax), a member of the corvid family. During their early life stages, ravens aggregate in large, mixed-sex, non-breeder groups of varying and inconsistent membership. During the day, they forage in temporary parties of varying sizes and compositions, ranging from as few as two subjects to groups of 20 or even 100 [16,17,18,19]. At night some join others to roost in large groups (up to hundreds of individuals). It is during this non-breeder stage that the formation, break-up, and re-formation of bonds and alliances occurs most frequently [19, 20]. Once they reach sexual maturity, at three years, ravens may form long-term pairs, leave other non-breeders, and attempt to occupy a breeding territory of their own, which they defend against other aspiring breeding pairs and groups of non-breeders [21].
When confronted with potential predators, corvids produce harsh alarm vocalisations directed at the predator (“scolding”), presumably both to harass the predator into leaving, and to recruit conspecifics for social support [2]. Such group mobbing can provide learning opportunities for inexperienced individuals [22], and has been shown to indicate alarm callers’ status in several corvid species (white-throated magpie-jays (Calocitta formosa) [23]; hooded crows (Corvus cornix) [24]; black-billed magpies (Pica hudsonia) [25]). While for common ravens this has yet to be shown, we know that individuality is encoded in other raven call types [26,27,28], and that ravens respond more strongly to alarm calls of adults, than those of juveniles [29].
In a series of elegant studies on wild American crows (Corvus brachyrhynchos), Marzluff and colleagues [13, 30] demonstrated that social learning about the potential threat of particular humans occurs, and transmits both horizontally within the local population, and vertically across generations. In those studies, human experimenters could be distinguished via facial masks, and their threat level was manipulated via their initial participation in, or absence from, catching and banding of crows. Using a similar design, we previously demonstrated that members of two captive groups of ravens can remember a ‘dangerous’ human for multiple years [31]. Interestingly, individuals showed considerable variation in their scolding response, and dominance status was a strong predictor for their behaviour. Indeed, dominant individuals (individuals that won the majority of their conflict interactions) took the lead in most scolding bouts, together with their closest affiliates, indicating strong social dynamics effects [31].
But why should dominant ravens differ from subordinates in scolding? A recent study on jackdaws found that the more individuals give an anti-predator response, the more attractive the display becomes to others to join [32] and, presumably, the more likely the predator is to leave. Given that ravens would profit from recruiting conspecifics to participate in anti-predator defence in similar ways, the described dominance-related variation in scolding seems puzzling. One possibility is that, in our previous study [31], not all of the ravens were knowledgeable about the predator stimulus, and that subordinates in particular had not yet learned that the masked human “predator” represents a risk. Another possibility is that social dynamics influence scolding behaviour and although all ravens knew about the predator, some ravens’ responses were suppressed. Some individuals might have been “free-loading” on the anti-predation efforts of others, typically dominants [33].
It is also possible that dominant individuals could afford to show more scolding than subordinates, simply because they were in a better physical condition (see [7]). The ravens’ anti-predator behaviour could thus serve as an honest signal, indicating the callers’ quality (see [34, 35]). Another possibility is that dominants actively suppress calling in subordinates, to highlight or exaggerate their own quality. Preventing others from calling is both energetically costly and takes time away from engaging in the ongoing anti-predator response, thus counteracting the beneficial effects of group mobbing. Hence, such a costly behaviour should occur only in low- to moderate-risk situations, and/or when potential mates are in the audience. Similar status-signalling effects have also been hypothesized for raven recruitment calls at rich but defended food sources [36], where high-status individuals within the non-breeder flock tend to produce more calls.
In the current study, we experimentally investigated the potential effect of such social dynamics on individual variation in ravens’ scolding behaviour. We followed up on our previous study, in which we trained two groups of eight ravens each to recognize a human wearing a particular mask (Fig. 1) as a potential novel “predator” [31]. During training, the masked person carried a dead raven in their hand, simulating the outcome of a predation event [37]. However, all subsequent test trials were carried out with the masked person only, and without any dead raven. One bird was excluded due to health issues, but the remaining 15 individuals were tested in both group and individual settings. Specifically, we compared scolding responses during six group trials, where motivational levels might be heavily impacted by social dynamics, to the responses in a single separation trial per individual, where any direct social interactions were absent. We based our hypotheses on the considerations mentioned above, specifying effects due to individual learning (or not) and social influences (or their absence). Our two hypotheses are:
-
Hypothesis 1: Low scolding durations by some individuals while in the group are not caused by social dynamics, but based on a failure to learn, resulting in some individuals simply not perceiving the artificial predator as a threat.
-
Hypothesis 2: Individuals with low scolding durations in the group did learn to recognize the artificial predator as a threat, but their scolding expression is decreased due to social dynamics, specifically their low rank.
Hypothesis 1 predicts the same pattern of calling, in both the group and separation trials, because failure to learn during the group trials would persist into the separation trials, leading to weak or no scolding responses there. Hypothesis 2 predicts different scolding patterns in the separation trial, where previously quiet subjects now would scold with more intensity, because the social dynamics preventing calling in the group condition would be absent in separation.