Skip to main content

Cranial morphology of captive mammals: a meta-analysis

Abstract

Background

Captive facilities such as zoos are uniquely instrumental in conservation efforts. To fulfill their potential as bastions for conservation, zoos must preserve captive populations as appropriate proxies for their wild conspecifics; doing so will help to promote successful reintroduction efforts. Morphological changes within captive populations may be detrimental to the fitness of individual animals because these changes can influence functionality; thus, it is imperative to understand the breadth and depth of morphological changes occurring in captive populations. Here, we conduct a meta-analysis of scientific literature reporting comparisons of cranial measures between captive and wild populations of mammals. We investigate the pervasiveness of cranial differences and whether cranial morphological changes are associated with ecological covariates specific to individual species, such as trophic level, dietary breadth, and home range size.

Results

Cranial measures of skull length, skull width, and the ratio of skull length-to-width differed significantly between many captive and wild populations of mammals reported in the literature. Roughly half of captive populations differed from wild populations in at least one cranial measure, although the degree of changes varied. Carnivorous species with a limited dietary breadth displayed the most consistent changes associated with skull widening. Species with a more generalized diet displayed less morphological changes in captivity.

Conclusions

Wild and captive populations of mammals differed in cranial morphology, but the nature and magnitude of their cranial differences varied considerably across taxa. Although changes in cranial morphology occur in captivity, specific changes cannot be generalized for all captive mammal populations. The nature of cranial changes in captivity may be specific to particular taxonomic groups; thus, it may be possible to establish expectations across smaller taxonomic units, or even disparate groups that utilize their cranial morphology in a similar way. Given that morphological changes occurring in captive environments like zoos have the potential to limit reintroduction success, our results call for a critical evaluation of current captive husbandry practices to prevent unnecessary morphological changes.

Background

Captive facilities such as zoos are important hubs for in situ and ex situ conservation where animals are often maintained in an effort to preserve species as faithful representatives of their wild counterparts so that captive populations can sustain the functionality and fitness of the species and perhaps, one day, be considered for reintroduction [39, 64, 75, 92, 117]. However, morphological changes occurring in a captive population may reduce the fitness of individual animals given that cranial morphology largely confers functionality [57, 74, 109]. It is therefore crucial to understand the breadth and depth of the morphological changes occurring in captive populations.

The skulls of captive mammals may differ from wild populations in both size and shape (e.g., [46, 88, 110, 120]). Documented differences include changes in the cranial length and width of African lions (Panthera leo [43, 46];), sagittal crest height of Amur tigers (P. tigris [28];), and mandibular morphology of Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata [50];), traits which are integral for feeding and influence bite force and dietary niche [77, 79, 108, 122]. The relative spread of the zygomatic arch is highly indicative of cranial musculature and functionality, where a wider zygomatic arch implies the presence of enhanced musculature and a stronger bite force often associated with carnivores (e.g., [24, 43]) and gnawing rodents (e.g., [30]). Although morphological changes in captivity have been reported in the literature, the nature (i.e., directionality and magnitude) of cranial differences and the ecological factors that may drive these differences (i.e., ecological covariates) have remained unclear.

The morphological differentiation of captive populations from their wild counterparts may be the result of inbreeding or evolutionary processes (e.g., genetic drift, selection), phenotypic plasticity (the ability of genotypes to display multiple, environmentally dependent phenotypes), or some combination of these factors [27, 33, 64, 113, 121]. Correlations have been found between a decrease in body size and inbreeding among captive wolves [44, 47, 58]. Molecular signatures of inbreeding and genetic drift have been noted among white-footed mice (Peromyscus leucopus) maintained in captivity [114, 115]. Lynch & Hayden [63] suggested the cranial changes they observed among farmed American mink (Mustela vison) were largely the result of differing selection pressures. Abnormal skull morphology of several captive mammals, including coyotes (Canis latrans [24];), African lions (Panthera leo [43];), and Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata [50];) have all largely been attributed to phenotypic plasticity.

In captivity, unusual phenotypes, especially of the crania, may be expressed as a plastic response to environmental factors related to novel diet textures [2446], nutrient availability [61, 103], or any other factors unique to the captive environment [40, 41, 87]. Cranial responses to a captive environment may be explained by differences in muscle usage, which may impact osteological traits [17, 87, 117, 119]. For example, a soft diet requires less musculature and therefore less mechanical stress is applied to the cranial bones, potentially resulting in a bone whose difference is greater than the variance of the mean wild-type morphotype [17, 46, 117]. Reduced mechanical constraint is also associated with reduced covariation between internal and external cranial morphology [24]. Therefore, a species whose wild diet is composed of particularly tough items may be more prone to morphological changes in captivity if captive diets are softer than what the animal might consume in the wild [24, 46, 51].

In addition to diet texture, the shape of cranial bones may be influenced by other factors related to captivity, such as stereotypic behaviors. Stereotypies are quite common among captive animals, yet they are rare in wild populations [69, 70]. Stereotypies are repetitive behaviors that serve no obvious function [69]; however, they may impact morphology due to the frequent, abnormal muscle usage involved in their performance [28, 43, 88, 99]. Stereotypies can include normal behaviors performed to the point of self-destruction (e.g., licking, grooming, rubbing) as well as head swinging, bar-biting, and pacing [69, 70, 72, 73]. Stereotypic overgrooming, for example, has been correlated with changes in the cranial morphology of captive tigers, where captive individuals display malformed sagittal crests associated with the heightened muscle usage involved in incessant grooming behaviors [28]. Stereotypic behaviors tend to be most common among captive animals with large wild home ranges [19, 56, 71] and those with highly specialized diets or food acquisition behaviors [65, 69, 73].

The degree to which morphology differs in captivity compared to wild populations may vary among species. For example, while African lions tend to show rather drastic, consistent morphological changes associated with an increase in zygomatic breadth [43, 46, 125], house mice (Mus musculus) show little morphological change in captivity [23]. Even closely related taxa may differ in the degree of change that they exhibit once in captivity [38, 50, 97], possibly due to species ecology where certain traits may predispose a particular species to a specific captive response. The likelihood of morphological changes occurring in captivity may increase when an animal’s habitat is difficult to replicate (leading to heightened stress behaviors) or when diets are difficult to accommodate [18, 24, 56]. Hypercarnivory (a diet that consists of a minimum of ~ 70% vertebrate prey [25, 109];), for instance, may predispose species to more extreme morphological differentiation in captivity [24, 43]. This is because skull shape is strongly linked to dietary function among wild carnivores [100, 109] and diet in captivity may be drastically different than it is in the wild [51]. Similarly, species that consume large prey have comparatively round skulls, where bowed zygomatic arches and heightened sagittal crests enable enhanced musculature and increased jaw strength [30, 100, 109]. If appropriate diets are not provided, differentiation in cranial morphology may occur in captivity [22, 24, 43, 46].

While the effects of captivity on mammalian cranial morphology are a recurrent theme in morphological research, the design of these studies vary, making it difficult to draw substantive and comprehensive conclusions about the nature of morphological changes occurring in captivity. The literature is generally limited to case studies of single species, several closely related species, or computational models predicting phenotypic trajectories. Here, we use a phylogenetic meta-analysis to examine effects reported in the existing literature and whether there are consistent differences in cranial morphological changes to help identify characteristics of species at the greatest risk of morphological change in captivity. In particular, we focused on studies reporting skull length, skull width, and the ratio of skull length to width, as these traits are intimately linked to cranial size and functionality [31, 60]. The magnitude and directionality of morphological changes among captive populations are expected to vary based on ecological factors, where the largest morphological changes are likely to occur among species whose diets and habitats are particularly difficult to accommodate in captivity (e.g., large home range size, carnivorous, narrow dietary breadth). Given that they may be responding to similar captive stressors, these species are also expected to display similar morphological changes (e.g., wider zygomatic breadth). By examining these hypotheses and developing a more comprehensive understanding of morphological changes that occur in captivity, effects may be reduced with updated husbandry practices to help ensure the long-term maintenance of captive populations and their potential for reintroduction success.

Results

Literature search

An exhaustive literature search with key words “zoo”, “captive”, “mammal”, “animal”, “skull”, “cranium”, “morphology”, and “size” revealed 515 potentially relevant publications examining differences in the cranial morphology of a population of captive mammals, of which 17 met the complete search criteria (examining a non-domesticated species in captive populations that have not experienced intentional artificial selection) and included all applicable data required for inclusion in at least one of the size or shape-related trait analyses (Table 1). The dates of the studies included in these analyses ranged from 1894 to 2020; however, methods used in most studies (caliper measurements) have not changed considerably over the past century. In total, these 17 publications included 47 comparative relationships between wild and captive populations (15 shape and 32 size-related variables), across 21 species, representing six mammalian orders. The most well-represented groups included carnivorans (n = 6) and primates (n = 5), although our meta-analyses also included ungulates (n = 4), rodents (n = 4), and two marsupials (Table 1).

Table 1 Studies and species included in each meta-analysis

In total, our study consisted of six meta-analyses, including analyses of size (skull length and width) and shape (skull length-to-width) to examine the magnitude and directionality of changes. For each assessment of cranial size and shape, data were analyzed with and without an absolute value applied to the standard effect sizes (a standardized statistic that encodes quantitative data from multiple studies into a common form [62];). Each of the meta-analyses were conducted as independent models with and without the inclusion of ecological covariates (trophic level, dietary breadth, home range size; Table S1), which may influence the degree of morphological differentiation between captive populations and their wild counterparts. Each of our meta-analyses included a slightly different collection of taxa based on taxa composition and their measures reported in each publication (Table 1). Publication bias has the potential to over-inflate the significance of meta-analytic models potentially leading to Type 1 errors, given studies that recover significant results may be more likely to be published [16, 62]. However, we did not detect publication bias in any of our meta-analyses (Fig. S1).

A strong phylogenetic signal was recovered in analyses of directionality associated with skull length (λ=0.89) and skull length-to-width (λ=0.88). Analyses of the magnitude of change did not recover a strong phylogenetic signal (Table 2).

Table 2 Pagel’s λ results to detect a phylogenetic signal

Morphological changes in captivity

Roughly half of species (11 of 21) displayed distinct trait values in captive populations in at least one of the analyses (and in at least one of the sexes, when more than one sex was reported). Skull length and width changes were often apparent among rodents and carnivores (including species in order Carnivora as well as the striped-faced dunnart, Sminthopsis macroura, a carnivorous marsupial; Fig. 1). These changes were generally not detected among primates and were only apparent in the skull length of gorillas and female vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus aethiops) (Fig. 1a). Several carnivorous species displayed changes related to an elongation and widening of the skull (e.g., Canis lupus, Panthera leo, S. macroura), while others (e.g., Acinonyx jubatus and Mustela nigripes) displayed the opposite pattern with a shorter, narrower cranium in captivity (Fig. 1a and b). Most rodents included in this study (e.g., Microtus arvalis, Myodes glareolus, and Peromyscus polionotus) displayed a significant increase in cranial width within the captive populations compared to their wild counterparts (Fig. 1b and c), while captive capybara (Hydrochoerus hydrochaeris) displayed an elongation and narrowing of the cranium (Fig. 1c). When sex was reported, similar morphological trends were typically displayed between both sexes within species, with some exceptions (e.g., female vervet monkeys, Chlorocebus aethiops displayed significantly longer crania in captive populations, although males did not; Fig. 1a). We did not detect a significant relationship between any of the directional changes in cranial morphology observed in captivity and the ecological covariates included in this study (trophic level, dietary breadth, and home range size; Fig. 2).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Cranial difference between captive and wild populations of individual mammal species. Forest plot of each meta-analysis for a: skull length, b: skull width, and c: skull length-to-width. Phylogenies shown to the left of each meta-analysis plot were pruned from Upham et al. [105]. Mammalian orders are indicated by color on the phylogeny: red=Carnivora, green=Perissodactyla, blue=Primates, purple=Dasyuromorpha, yellow=Diprotodontia, and gray=Rodentia (see Table 1 for additional taxonomic information). Colors within the plot indicate the sex of the specimens from each study: magenta=females, blue=males, and green indicates a study that used a pooled sample of both sexes. Summary effect sizes for directional analyses as well as analyses of magnitude are indicated in black. Forest plot lines that do not cross the dotted zero line are associated with a significant effect (i.e., the effect is not zero). Red arrows on the skull illustrations indicate the morphology associated with the positive and negative effect sizes on each plot

Fig. 2
figure 2

Analyses of directionality associated with ecological covariates. a-c: trophic level, d-f: dietary breadth, and g-i: relative home range size. Columns indicate analyses of skull length (a, d, g), skull width (b, e, h), and skull length-to-width (c, f, i). Forest plot lines that do not cross the dotted zero line are associated with a significant effect. Red arrows on the skull illustrations indicate the morphology associated with the positive and negative effect sizes on each plot

Phylogenetic meta-analysis models of the magnitude of change detected moderate to strong effects (r) for skull length (r = 0.35, p = 1.3e-9***), skull width (r = 0.45, p = 3.7e-3**), and skull length-to-width (r = 0.36, p = 4.0e-3**; Table 3); 0.3 < r < 0.5 is interpreted as a moderate effect and r > 0.5 is considered a strong effect in the ecological literature [35, 83]. Analyses of magnitude also revealed moderate to strong effects associated with the carnivorous trophic level (skull length, r = 0.38, p = 5.7e-7***; skull width, r = 0.71, p = 3.0e-3**; skull length-to-width, r = 0.39, p = 0.03*; Table 3; Fig. 3a-c), the narrowest dietary breadth (skull length, r = 0.36, p = 9.1e-6***; skull width, r = 0.69, p = 1.9e-4***; skull length-to-width, r = 0.36, p = 0.03*; Table 3; Fig. 3d-f), and the smallest home range size (skull length, r = 0.39, p = 2.3e-6***; skull width, r = 0.49, p = 0.01*; skull length-to-width, r = 0.55, p = 2.0e-4***; Table 3; Fig. 3g-i). Phylogenetic meta-analysis models of the magnitude of skull length also revealed strong effects associated with herbivory (r = 0.39, p = 4.7e-3**; Table 3; Fig. 3a) and a moderate dietary breadth (r = 0.41, p = 1.2e-4***; Table 3; Fig. 3d).

Table 3 Results for all full model and ecological covariate meta-analyses
Fig. 3
figure 3

Analyses of magnitude associated with ecological covariates. a-c: trophic level, d-f: dietary breadth, and g-i: relative home range size. Columns indicate analyses of skull length (a, d, g), skull width (b, e, h), or skull length-to-width (c, f, i). Forest plot lines that do not cross the dotted zero line demonstrate a significant difference between captive and wild populations

Discussion

Captive facilities such as zoos can provide an environment for breeding and maintenance of threatened or endangered species that may otherwise exist in perilous conditions [4, 10, 21]; however, questions regarding the morphological differentiation of captive populations have caused concern that these facilities may not preserve species as appropriate proxies for the wild population. The results of our phylogenetic meta-analyses of magnitude indicate that although directionality of the morphological changes occurring in captivity varied between taxa, the overall magnitude of standard effect size was consistently different from zero. This suggests the mammals examined in our study display size and shape changes in captivity, but they do not all differ in the same way. Although analyses that use absolute values have been shown to inflate model significance and create artificially narrow confidence intervals [84, 91], we chose to analyze these data because the direction of change in traits appeared inconsistent, yet there were many trait-species combinations that had no 95% confidence overlap with zero, indicating change, regardless of direction, may be indicative of populations in captivity. Applying absolute values to standard effect sizes neutralizes directionality and can therefore help interpret whether there are any general effects of captivity, particularly when extreme positive and negative values exist in the dataset.

The differentiation in cranial morphology between captive and wild populations of rodents, carnivorans, and the carnivorous marsupial S. macroura may be related to the well-developed temporalis muscles that these species possess [30]. Temporalis muscles enhance jaw strength at the anterior portion of the skull, which translates to enhanced incisor gnawing strength among rodents and increased force distributed to the canine teeth of carnivorous mammals [30]. The presence of an enlarged temporalis muscle requires a common set of morphological specializations including a wider zygomatic arch (i.e., a wider skull [30];). Species that do not heavily rely on anterior jaw strength, such as most herbivores and omnivores, typically display a narrower zygomatic breadth (i.e., a narrower skull [30];), as we saw in this study (Fig. 2b). The cranial shapes observed in captivity may seem counterintuitive, where captive specimens of several species display wider skulls than their wild counterparts, with documented instances of correspondingly enhanced cranial musculature [87] and bite force [32]. This seemingly contradictory result may be related, at least in part, to the changes in muscle usage that occur in captivity [40, 41, 87]. While the pressure to capture prey and flee predators may be removed in captivity, certain captivity-specific tasks, such as the performance of stereotypic behaviors and the processing of novel diets may also increase muscle usage and may explain the morphological differences observed in this study [40, 41, 87, 98]. Similarly, wild diets may constrain cranial shapes to maintain optimum functionality for processing and capturing prey items, particularly among species with highly specialized diets [2, 32, 60, 109]. In captivity, the absence of these natural selection pressures may lead to a greater distribution of mean trait values or a loss in the covariation of cranial modules [24, 74, 76].

Trophic level, dietary breadth, and home range size were all associated with changes in morphology in captive animals. As noted above, carnivorous species tended to display morphological change in captivity; however, trophic level was broadly categorized with only carnivorous, omnivorous, and herbivorous groupings. A more detailed examination of dietary categories including subcategories associated with folivory, insectivory, or granivory, for example, may provide a more nuanced exploration of the role trophic level plays in the morphological change observed in captivity. Species with the largest home range sizes in the wild frequently display heightened stereotypies in captivity [19, 56, 71] and possible subsequent morphological change compared to wild populations, although some of the most distinct morphological shifts we observed were associated with species that inhabit the smallest home range sizes (e.g., M. nigripes; Fig. 1). However, it may be that this finding is more strongly associated with trophic levels given that some of the species that displayed the largest morphological changes are carnivorous and also have some of the smallest home range sizes.

The specific morphological changes associated with captivity varied within mammalian orders. For example, while the majority of rodents included in this study (Microtus arvalis, Myodes glareolus, and Peromyscus polionotus) displayed a wider cranium in captivity (Fig. 1b and c), the zoo population of capybaras (Hydrochaeris hydrochaeris) displayed the opposite trend, with a narrower, more elongated cranium in captivity. Capybara are distinct from the other rodent species included in this study in several ways. With an average body weight of roughly 50 kg, capybara are the largest rodent species and the only members of Family Caviidae included in this study (all other rodents in this study are members of Family Cricetidae; Table 1). Unlike many rodent species, capybara also have a semi-aquatic lifestyle and as selective grazing herbivores that consume primarily grass, they fill a dietary niche that is more similar to cattle than it is to many other rodent species [11, 49]. The lifestyle and dietary differences between capybara and the other rodent species included in this study may help to explain the opposing morphological trends that we observed. Likewise, while captive gray wolves (Canis lupus) displayed longer crania in captivity, captive cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus) and black-footed ferrets (Mustela nigripes) displayed notably shorter crania, although all three species are members of Order Carnivora. The reasons for this difference within a relatively close taxonomic unit are unclear but may be related to the extensive population bottlenecks experienced by the latter species [80, 116]. While certain morphological changes occurring in captivity have been attributed to selection [64, 115] or phenotypic plasticity [40, 87] for traits that may be advantageous in the captive environment, species with especially depleted genetic lines may display less favorable morphological traits. However, this topic requires further investigation into the mechanisms that drive these changes and the relative favorability of these traits in captivity.

Although studies examining the morphological effects of captivity often assess exclusively adult specimens, mammalian cranial morphology may differ significantly between older and younger adult specimens, where certain skull proportions, such as facial length, width, and mandibular length may continue to change throughout an adult animal’s lifetime [3, 106, 107]. Within human populations, for example, the cranial elements of a person at age 30 can differ significantly from those at age 80 [3, 48]. Animals maintained in captivity may experience a longer lifespan than those living in the wild [26, 53] and be subject to additional morphological change. It is unclear how these age-related morphological changes may influence differentiation of captive and wild populations. Specific age information was largely unavailable in the studies included in these meta-analyses, although nearly all specimens were designated as adults (aside from two studies that did not report age information and one study that included three ‘nearly mature’ specimens, see the methods section), thus we were unable to consider this topic more closely. Future research on the effect of age on morphology should examine whether the increased lifespan frequently found in captivity may act as a confounding factor in the morphological differentiation observed between these populations.

Captive animals often have well-documented histories, exist in highly controlled environments, and, in some cases, represent the largest accessible populations of rare or endangered species [13, 14, 101, 104] and are thus ideal for biological research, especially studies focusing on morphological changes over time compared to wild populations. However, our findings suggest that certain specimens from captive populations should be preferentially avoided in morphological research, particularly carnivorous species and rodents, which can display distinct morphological shifts in captivity. In contrast, captive primates and other omnivorous species appear to show negligible shifts in cranial size and shape. This supports the findings of Bello-Hellegouarch et al. [14], whose geometric morphometric study of great ape scapula found similarly limited differences between wild and captive populations. Our findings suggest that researchers examining some species, or anatomical regions unlikely to be impacted by captivity, need not avoid captive specimens in future morphological studies.

Conclusions

Changes in cranial morphology of captive mammals may impact dietary function and limit the conservation potential of captive populations [6, 74, 92, 114, 117]. Although differences in cranial morphology of captive mammals has long been recognized, the nature and commonality of those differences have been poorly understood. The findings of our phylogenetic meta-analyses suggest that differences in mammalian cranial morphology occur in captivity, but the nature and magnitude of those differences often varies among species. The overall magnitude of these differences implies that further investigation within individual species and at higher taxonomic levels is warranted to better understand how and why cranial morphology changes in captivity compared to wild mammal populations, especially studies examining the evolutionary mechanisms of these morphological changes. As captive facilities such as zoos become increasingly responsible for the long-term survival of threatened and endangered species [21, 112, 118], developing an understanding of the morphological changes occurring in captivity will be essential to avoid these effects in the future.

Methods

We conducted phylogenetic meta-analyses to examine differences in cranial morphology of captive mammals compared to their wild counterparts documented in previously published literature. We focused on three traits: skull length and skull width (traits associated with size), and the ratio of skull length-to-width (a trait associated with shape).

Literature search and meta-analysis

We conducted an exhaustive search of the literature using search functions in Web of Science, Google Scholar, and PQDT Open. Searches were conducted using the key terms, ‘zoo’ or ‘captive’, ‘mammal’ or ‘animal’, and ‘skull’, ‘cranium’, ‘morphology’ or ‘size’ and were completed in August 2020. Additional studies were located by searching the reference sections of literature on the topic. Literature searches were refined to only include studies which 1) provided comparative size and/or shape data of captive and wild mammals, 2) assessed non-domesticated captive species (as described in [72], following the species listed in [59, 123]), and 3) assessed captive populations that had not experienced intentional artificial selection.

Captive facilities included zoos, laboratories, or other breeding centers. Animals that were bred for specific traits (e.g., farm populations bred for size, laboratory colonies bred for particular attributes) were excluded from these analyses. Efforts were made to assess exclusively adult animals; exceptions to this included three of six captive female Dicerorhinus sumatrensis specimens, which were suggested to be ‘nearly mature’ [37] and two studies that did not specify the age of the specimens that were analyzed [5, 37]. In studies examining multiple age groups (e.g. [2]), only the data from the adult categories were included. We downloaded data associated with the publications and when relevant data were unavailable with the publication, we contacted the corresponding author with a request to share available data. If data could not be obtained, these studies were removed from analyses (n = 3). All analyses followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement guidelines, which provides recommendations for the inclusion of studies in meta-analyses, as well as parameters for data extraction [82].

We conducted six phylogenetic meta-analyses, including analyses of size (skull length and width) and shape (skull length-to-width) to examine both the magnitude and directionality of changes for mammalian populations in captivity compared to their wild counterparts. We describe the ratio of skull length-to width as a shape using the most general definition of the term in which the ratio of two linear measures provides an approximation of a structure’s shape [55, 85]. Each of our meta-analyses included a different collection of taxa based on the measures reported in each publication (Table 1). We used standard effect sizes (a standardized statistic that encodes quantitative data from multiple studies into a common form [62];) and the absolute value of standard effect sizes in our meta-analyses. We also conducted meta-analyses with the inclusion of ecological covariates (trophic level, dietary breadth, home range size; Table S1).

To assess publication bias, the tendency for significant results to be disproportionally published [15, 93], we used funnel plots that display the distribution of standard effect sizes with corresponding variances. Asymmetry in a funnel plot is indicative of publication bias, whereas an unbiased sample will produce a relatively conical pattern of points. Egger’s regression (mixed-effect meta-regression model) was used to assess asymmetry in each funnel plot [2986, 102].

Standard effect size

Data including sample size, mean, and standard deviation or standard error were extracted from each study and used to calculate a standard effect size for each variable (including skull length, skull width, and skull length-to-width; Table S2). When no variance measures were provided [37, 38], the prognostic method, a conservative estimate of missing variance terms, was applied to estimate missing standard deviations (Table S2) using the sample size and variance data available in the other studies included in the dataset (see the following for a review of these methods: [66, 67]). To calculate the corresponding standard effect size of shape ratios, pooled standard deviations were calculated based on 10,000 permutations using the sample size, mean, and standard deviation of both linear measures. When necessary, measurements were extracted from publication figures using MorphoJ ([54]; Table S2), which provides an estimate of the x- and y- coordinates of every point in a scatterplot. These values were then used to calculate the mean and standard deviation of the measures associated with captive and wild populations in the study. To assess the effects across studies, Pearson’s correlation coefficient (r) was calculated as a measure of the standard effect size [20] and converted to Fisher’s Z, a normality transformation typically applied to meta-analyses [1, 1296].

Both traditional (n = 15) and geometric morphometric (n = 2) studies were assessed in these analyses (Table S2). Linear measures found in traditional morphometric studies were used to estimate skull length and skull width. To derive shape variables from traditional morphometric studies, ratios of linear measures (i.e., skull length to skull width) were taken. Skull shapes were extracted from geometric morphometric studies using principal component (PC) scores reported in the publications. As a rigid rotation, principal component analyses preserve the covariation between specimens, where PC scores represent independent axes of shape variation [81, 124] and were used exclusively in the meta-analysis of cranial shape (skull length-to-width). Specific shapes represented by each PC was determined from publication text and figures depicting morphology (e.g., thin-plate splines). When necessary (only for Peromyscus polionotus, [74]) the extracted PC scores were multiplied by − 1 to preserve the relative skull length-to-width relationship between the captive and wild populations described in the manuscript text, given the relative ordination of the PCA provided in the publication.

Ecological covariates

We assessed the ecological covariates trophic level, dietary breadth, and home range size (assessed in wild populations) to evaluate the ways in which species ecology may be associated with changes in morphology in captive populations (Table S1). These ecological data were derived from the open-access PanTHERIA dataset [49]. Trophic levels included carnivorous, omnivorous, and herbivorous, which are broadly defined in the PanTHERIA dataset by the presence or absence of vertebrate or non-vertebrate prey in an animal’s diet. A carnivorous species is defined by its nearly exclusive consumption of either vertebrate or invertebrate prey, herbivorous species are defined by not consuming any prey items, and omnivorous species are defined by their consumption of mixed dietary categories. Dietary breadth accounts for the number of dietary categories consumed by a species and ranges from one to eight in the PanTHERIA dataset. For our analyses, dietary breadth was parsed into three categories, including species that consume items from a single dietary category, those consuming two to three dietary categories, and those consuming four or more dietary categories. Lastly, home range sizes in the wild were recorded as the average area inhabited by a species (km2); these values were converted to categorical variables (small, 0.01–5 km2; medium, 11–30 km2; and large, 55–160 km2) based on the distribution of these values in this dataset. Home range sizes were estimated from additional sources when data were not available for a given species in PanTHERIA, including Asiatic wild asses (Equus hemionus [36];), vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus aethiops [45];), European pine voles (Microtus subterraneus), and stripe-faced dunnarts (Sminthopsis macroura) whose estimated home range size was inferred to be less than 0.1 km2 based on the data available for other members of the genus in PanTHERIA.

Phylogenetic non-Independence

We pruned phylogenetic trees inferred by Upham et al. [105] to species present in each phylogenetic meta-analysis using picante [1, 52]. Pagel’s λ was used to assess phylogenetic signal of standard effect size for each of the variables we evaluated [34, 89, 90] by assessing 2000 randomly selected phylogenies from Upham et al. [105] with geiger [42]. Pagel’s λ results are reported as the mean and standard deviation of all iterations (Table 3). A strong phylogenetic signal was recovered in analyses of skull length and skull length to width (see Results and Table 3). Thus, these analyses were conducted using phylogenetic comparative methods to account for evolutionary non-independence in our meta-analyses ([34, 35]; Table 3).

Multi-variate phylogenetic meta-analyses were conducted with Metafor [1, 93, 111]. Studies included in our meta-analyses reported values for single sexes, individual values for each sex, or pooled samples of both sexes. Thus, we incorporated a random effect variable of sex in meta-analytic models, in addition to other typical random effects variables of study and species [12, 111]. The ‘species’ random effect accounted for the uneven inclusion of species in the analyses and the phylogenetic covariance was specified by the correlation matrix [111]. The ‘study’ random effect accounted for the variation across individual studies. Each phylogenetic meta-analysis was conducted using 2000 randomly selected phylogenies from Upham et al. [105] and reported as mean and standard deviations of those iterations. Phylogenetic covariance was calculated for each phylogeny as described in Adams [1]. All analyses were conducted in R version 3.6.1 [94].

Availability of data and materials

All aggregated data analyzed during this study are included in this published article and its supplementary information files. Any additional data are available upon request to the corresponding author.

References

  1. Adams DC. Phylogenetic meta-analysis. Evolution. 2008;62:567–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Aeschbach M, Carrillo JD, Sánchez-Villagra MR. On the growth of the largest living rodent: postnatal skull and dental shape changes in capybara species (Hydrochoerus spp.). Mamm Biol. 2016;81(6):558–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Albert AM, Ricanek K Jr, Patterson E. A review of the literature on the aging adult skull and face: implications for forensic science research and applications. Forensic Sci Int. 2007;172(1):1–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Alroy J. Limits to captive breeding of mammals in zoos. Conserv Biol. 2015;29(3):926–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Antonelli T. The cranial morphology of the black-footed ferret: a comparison of wild and captive specimens. Columbia: University of South Carolina; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Araki H, Cooper B, Blouin MS. Genetic effects of captive breeding cause a rapid, cumulative fitness decline in the wild. Science. 2007;318:100–3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Balčiauskienė L. Cranial growth of captive bred common voles (Microtus arvalis). Acta Zool Litu. 2007;17(3):220–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Balčiauskienė. Cranial growth of captive bred bank voles (Clethrionomys glareolus). Acta Zool Litu. 2007b;17:33–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Balčiauskienė L, Balčiauskas L. Prediction of the body mass of the bank vole Myodes glareolus from skull measurements. Eston J Ecol. 2009;58(2):77–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Balmford A, Kroshko J, Leader-Williams N, Mason G, Conde DA, Flesness N, et al. Zoos and captive breeding. Science. 2011;332(3):1149–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Barreto GR, Quintana RD. Foraging strategies and feeding habits of capybaras. In: Moreira JR, Ferraz KMPMB, Herrera EA, Macdonald DW, editors. Capybara: biology, use and conservation of an exceptional Neotropical species. New York: Springer Science & Business Media; 2009. p. 83–96.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Becker DJ, Streicker DG, Altizer S. Using host species traits to understand the consequences of resource provisioning for host–parasite interactions. J Anim Ecol. 2018;87(2):511–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Behringer V, Stevens JMG, Deschner T, Hohmann G. Getting closer: contributions of zoo studies to research on the physiology and development of bonobos Pan paniscus, chimpanzees pan troglodytes and other primates. Int Zoo Yearb. 2018;52:34–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Bello-Hellegouarch G, Potau JM, Arias-Martorell J, Pastor JF, Pérez-Pérez A. Brief communication: morphological effects of captivity: a geometric morphometric analysis of the dorsal side of the scapula in captive-bred and wild-caught Hominoidea. Am J of Phys Anthropol. 2013;152(2):306–10.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Borenstein M, Cooper H, Hedges L, Valentine J. Effect sizes for continuous data. In: The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis (2nd ed); 2009. p. 221–35.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Borm GF, Donders ART. Updating meta-analyses leads to larger type I errors than publication bias. J Clin Epidemiol. 2009;62(8):825–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Cheronet O, Finarelli JA, Pinhasi R. Morphological change in cranial shape following the transition to agriculture across western Eurasia. Sci Rep. 2016;6:1–10.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Clubb R, Mason G. Captivity effects on wide-ranging carnivores. Nature. 2003;425(6957):473–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Clubb R, Rowcliffe M, Lee P, Mar KU, Moss C, Mason GJ. Fecundity and population viability in female zoo elephants: problems and possible solutions. Anim Welf. 2009;18(3):237–47.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Conde DA, Flesness N, Colchero F, Jones OR, Scheuerlein A. An emerging role of zoos to conserve biodiversity. Science. 2011;331:1390–1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Corruccini RS, Beecher RM. Occlusal variation related to soft diet in a nonhuman primate. Science. 1982;218:74–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Courtney Jones SK, Munn AJ, Byrne PG. Effect of captivity on morphology: negligible changes in external morphology mask significant changes in internal morphology. R Soc Open Sci. 2018;5(5):1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Curtis AA, Orke M, Tetradis S, Van Valkenburgh B. Diet-related differences in craniodental morphology between captive-reared and wild coyotes, Canis latrans (Carnivora: Canidae). Biol J Linn Soc. 2018;123(3):677–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Damasceno EM, Hingst-Zaher E, Astúa D. Bite force and encephalization in the Canidae (Mammalia: Carnivora). J Zool. 2013;290(4):246–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. de Magalhaes JP, Costa J. A database of vertebrate longevity records and their relation to other life-history traits. J Evol Biol. 2009;22(8):1770–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. DeWitt TJ, Scheiner SM, editors. Phenotypic plasticity: functional and conceptual approaches. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Duckler GL. An unusual osteological formation in the posterior skulls of captive tigers (Panthera tigris). Zoo Biol. 1998;17(2):135–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Egger M, Smith GD, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. Br Med J. 1997;315:629–34.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Elbroch M. Animal skulls: a guide to north American species. Mechanicsburg, PA: Stackpole Books; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Ellis JL, Thomason J, Kebreab E, Zubair K, France J. Cranial dimensions and forces of biting in the domestic dog. J Anat. 2009;214:362–73.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Erickson GM, Lappin AK, Parker T, Vliet KA. Comparison of bite-force performance between long-term captive and wild American alligators (Alligator mississippiensis). J Zool. 2004;262:21–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Frankham R. Genetic adaptation to captivity in species conservation programs. Mol Ecol. 2008;17:325–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Freckleton RP, Harvey PH, Pagel M. Phylogenetic analysis and comparative data: a test and review of evidence. Am Nat. 2002;160(6):712–26.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Garamszegi LZ, Markó G, Herczeg G. A meta-analysis of correlated behaviours with implications for behavioural syndromes: mean effect size, publication bias, phylogenetic effects and the role of mediator variables. Evol Ecol. 2012;26(5):1213–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Giotto N, Gerard JF, Ziv A, Bouskila A, Bar-David S. Space-use patterns of the Asiatic wild ass (Equus hemionus): Complementary insights from displacement, recursion movement and habitat selection analyses. PLoS One. 2015;10(12):e0143279.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Groves CP. Skull-changes due to captivity in certain Equidae. Z Säugietierkd. 1966;31:44–6.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Groves CP. The skulls of Asian rhinoceroses: wild and captive. Zoo Biol. 1982;1:251–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Guay PJ, Parrott M, Selwood L. Captive breeding does not alter brain volume in a marsupial over a few generations. Zoo Biol. 2012;31:82–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Harbers H, Neaux D, Ortiz K, Blanc B, Laurens F, Baly I, Callou C, Schafberg R, et al. The mark of captivity: plastic responses in the ankle bone of a wild ungulate (Sus scrofa). R Soc Open Sci. 2020a;7(3):192039.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Harbers H, Zanolli C, Cazenave M, Theil J-C, Ortiz K, Blanc B, Locatelli Y, Schafberg R, et al. Investigating the impact of captivity and domestication on limb bone cortical morphology: an experimental approach using a wild boar model. Sci Rep. 2020b;10(1):1–13.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Harmon LJ, Weir JT, Brock CD, Glor RE, Challenger W. GEIGER: investigating evolutionary radiations. Bioinform. 2008;24:129–31.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Hartstone-Rose A, Selvey H, Villari JR, Atwell M, Schmidt T. The three-dimensional morphological effects of captivity. PLoS One. 2014;9(11):1–15.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Hedrick PW, Fredrickson R. Genetic rescue guidelines with examples from Mexican wolves and Florida panthers. Conserv Genet. 2010;11(2):615–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Herzog NM, Parker CH, Keefe ER, Coxworth J, Barrett A, Hawkes K. Fire and home range expansion: a behavioral response to burning among savanna dwelling vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus aethiops). Am J Phys Anthropol. 2014;154(4):554–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Hollister N. Some effects of environment and habit on captive lions. Proc US Natl Mus. 1917;53(2196):177–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Ibanez B, Moreno E, Barbosa A. No inbreeding effects on body size in two captive endangered gazelles. Mamm Biol. 2011;76:748–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Israel H. The dichotomous pattern of craniofacial expansion during aging. Am J Phys Anthropol. 1977;47:47–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Jones KE, Bielby J, Cardillo M, Fritz SA, O’Dell J, Orme CDL, et al. PanTHERIA: a species-level database of life history, ecology, and geography of extant and recently extinct mammals. Ecol. 2009;90(9):2648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Kamaluddin SN, Tanaka M, Wakamori H, Nishimura T, Ito T. Phenotypic plasticity in the mandibular morphology of Japanese macaques: captive – wild comparison. R Soc Open Sci. 2019;6(7):181382.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Kapoor V, Antonelli T, Parkinson JA, Hartstone-Rose A. Oral health correlates of captivity. Res Vet Sci. 2016;107:213–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Kembel SW, Cowan PD, Helmus MR, Cornwell WK, Morlon H, Ackerly DD, et al. Picante: R tools for integrating phylogenies and ecology. Bioinform. 2010;26:1463–4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  53. Kitchener A, MacDonald A. The longevity legacy: the problem of old mammals in zoos. In: Proceedings of the EAZA Conference Amsterdam; 2004. p. 132–7.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Klingenberg CP, Morpho J. Faculty of Life Sciences, University of Manchester, vol. 3; 2008. p. 75–7.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Klingenberg CP. Size, shape, and form: concepts of allometry in geometric morphometrics. Dev Genes Evol. 2016;226(3):113–37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Kroshko J, Clubb R, Harper L, Mellor E, Moehrenschlager A, Mason G. Stereotypic route tracing in captive Carnivora is predicted by species-typical home range sizes and hunting styles. Anim Behav. 2016;117:197–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. La Croix S, Zelditch ML, Shivik JA, Lundrigan BL, Holekamp KE. Ontogeny of feeding performance and biomechanics in coyotes. J Zool. 2011;285(4):301–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Laikre L, Ryman N. Inbreeding depression in a captive wolf (Canis lupus) population. Conserv Biol. 1991;5:33–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Larson G, Burger J. A population genetics view of animal domestication. Trends Genet. 2013;29(4):197–205.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Law CJ, Duran E, Hung N, Richards E, Santillan I, Mehta RS. Effects of diet on cranial morphology and biting ability in musteloid mammals. J Evol Biol. 2018;31:1918–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Leigh SR. Relations between captive and noncaptive weights in anthropoid primates. Zoo Biol. 1994;13:21–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Lipsey MW, Wilson DB. Practical meta-analysis (Vol 49). Thousand oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Lynch JM, Hayden TJ. Genetic influences on cranial form: variation among ranch and feral American mink Mustela vison (Mammalia: Mustelidae). Biol J Linn Soc. 1995;55:293–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Lynch M, O’Hely M. Captive breeding and the genetic fitness of natural populations. Conserv Genet. 2001;2:363–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Lyons J, Young RJ, Deag JM. The effects of physical characteristics of the environment and feeding regime on the behavior of captive felids. Zoo Biol. 1997;16:71–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Ma J, Liu W, Hunter A, Zhang W. Performing meta-analysis with incomplete statistical information in clinical trials. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2008;8:1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Ma J, Liu W, Hunter A, Zhang W. An XML based framework for merging incomplete and inconsistent statistical information from clinical trials. In: Soft computing in XML data management. Berlin: Springer; 2010. p. 259–90.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  68. Markov G, Csorba G, Kocheva M, Gospodinova M. Skull features of the common vole (Microtus arvalis sensu lato) from Hungary: craniometrical evidence for its taxonomic detachment. Turk J Zool. 2012;36(3):283–90.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Mason G. Stereotypies: a critical review. Anim Behav. 1991;41:1015–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Mason GJ. Species differences in responses to captivity: stress, welfare and the comparative method. Trends Ecol Evol. 2010;25(12):713–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Mason GJ, Veasey JS. How should the psychological well-being of zoo elephants be objectively investigated? Zoo Biol. 2010;29(2):237–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Mason G, Burn CC, Dallaire JA, Kroshko J, McDonald Kinkaid H, Jeschke JM. Plastic animals in cages: Behavioural flexibility and responses to captivity. Anim Behav. 2013;85(5):1113–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. McPhee ME. Intact carcasses as enrichment for large felids: effects on on- and off-exhibit behaviors. Zoo Biol. 2002;21:37–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. McPhee ME. Morphological change in wild and captive Oldfield mice Peromyscus polionotus subgriseus. J Mammal. 2004;85(6):1130–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. McPhee ME, Carlstead K. The importance of maintaining natural behaviors in captive mammals. In: Kleiman DG, Allen M, Thompson K, editors. Wild mammals in captivity: principles and techniques for zoo management. 2nd ed. Chicago, Il: University of Chicago Press; 2010. p. 303–13.

    Google Scholar 

  76. McPhee ME, McPhee NF. Relaxed selection and environmental change decrease reintroduction success in simulated populations. Anim Conserv. 2012;15:274–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. McPhee ME, Silverman ED. Increased behavioral variation and the calculation of release numbers for reintroduction programs. Conserv Biol. 2004;18(3):705–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Meachen J, Schmidt-Küntzel A, Marker L. Regional variation in the cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus) revisited: morphology of wild and captive populations. Zoo Biol. 2020;39(2):83–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Meloro C, Hunter J, Tomsett L, Portela Miguez R, Prevosti FJ, Brown RP. Evolutionary ecomorphology of the Falkland Islands wolf Dusicyon australis. Mammal Rev. 2017;47(2):159–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Menotti-Raymond M, O’Brien SJ. Dating the genetic bottleneck of the African cheetah. PNAS. 1993;90(8):3172–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Mitteroecker P, Gunz P. Advances in geometric morphometrics. Evol Biol. 2009;36(2):235–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, GROUP P. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Ann Intern Med. 2009;151:264–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Møller A, Jennions MD. How much variance can be explained by ecologists and evolutionary biologists? Oecologia. 2002;132(4):492–500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Morrissey MB. Meta-analysis of magnitudes, differences and variation in evolutionary parameters. J Evol Biol. 2016;29(10):1882–904.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Mosimann JE. Size allometry: size and shape variables with characterizations of the lognormal and generalized gamma distributions. J Am Stat Assoc. 1970;65(330):930–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Murray MH, Sánchez CA, Becker DJ, Byers KA, Worsley-Tonks KEL, Craft ME. City sicker? A meta-analysis of wildlife health and urbanization. Front Ecol Environ. 2019;17(10):575–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Neaux D, Blanc B, Ortiz K, Locatelli Y, Laurens F, Baly I, Callou C, Lecompte F, et al. How changes in functional demands associated with captivity affect the skull shape of a wild boar (Sus scrofa). Evol Biol. 2020;0123456789:1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  88. O’Regan HJ, Kitchener AC. The effects of captivity on the morphology of captive, domesticated and feral mammals. Mamm Rev. 2005;35(3&4):215–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Pagel M. Detecting correlated evolution on phylogenies: a general method for the comparative analysis of discrete characters. P Roy Soc B-Biol Sci. 1994;255(1342):37–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Pagel M. Inferring evolutionary processes from phylogenies. Zool Scr. 1997;26(4):331–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  91. Paulus FM, Krach S, Albrecht AG, Jansen A. Potential bias in meta-analyses of effect sizes in imaging genetics. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39(3):501–3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  92. Pelletier F, Réale D, Watters J, Boakes EH, Garant D. Value of captive populations for quantitative genetics research. Trends Ecol Evol. 2009;24(5):263–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Quintana DS. From pre-registration to publication: a non-technical primer for conducting a meta-analysis to synthesize correlational data. Front Psychol. 2015;6:1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  95. Rose R. Reproductive biology of the Tasmanian bettong (Bettongia gaimardi). Hobart: University of Tasmania; 1984.

  96. Rosenthal R. Meta-analytic procedures for social science research. Educ Res. 1986;15(8):18–20.

    Google Scholar 

  97. Saragusty J, Shavit-Meyrav A, Yamaguchi N, Nadler R, Bdolah-Abram T, Gibeon L, et al. Comparative skull analysis suggests species-specific captivity-related malformation in lions (Panthera leo). PLoS ONE. 2014;9(4).

  98. Selvey HR. Cranial responses to captivity in Lemur catta and Propithecus verreauxi in Natural History Museum collections. Boulder: University of Colorado; 2015. p. 1–82.

    Google Scholar 

  99. Skibiel AL, Trevino HS, Naugher K. Comparison of several types of enrichment for captive felids. Zoo Biol. 2007;26(2):371–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Slater GJ, Dumont ER, Van Valkenburgh B. Implications of predatory specialization for cranial form and function in canids. J Zool. 2009;278(3):181–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  101. Spartaco G, Kitchener AC. The Italian zoological gardens and their role in mammal systematic studies, conservation biology and museum collections. Hystrix. 2007;18(2):173–84.

    Google Scholar 

  102. Sterne J, Egger M. Regression methods to detect publication and other bias in meta-analysis. In: Rothstein H, Sutton AJ, Borenstein M, editors. Publication bias in meta-analysis: prevention, assessment, and adjustments. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  103. Terranova CJ, Coffman BS. Body weights of wild and captive lemurs. Zoo Biology. 1997;16:17–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Turner TR, Cramer JD, Nisbett A, Gray JP. A comparison of adult body size between captive and wild vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus aethiops sabaeus) on the island of St. Kitts Primates. 2016;57(2):211–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Upham NS, Esselstyn JA, Jetz W. Inferring the mammal tree: species-level sets of phylogenies for questions in ecology, evolution, and conservation. PLOS Biol. 2019;17(2):e3000494.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  106. Urban JE, Weaver AA, Lillie EM, Maldjian JA, Whitlow CT, Stitzel JD. Evaluation of morphological changes in the adult skull with age and sex. J Anat. 2016;229(6):838–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Van Minh N, Mouri T, Hamada Y. Aging-related changes in the skulls of Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Anthropol Sci. 2015;123(2):107–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  108. Van Valkenburgh B. Carnivore dental adaptations and diet: A study of trophic diversity within guilds. In: Gittleman JL, editor. Carnivore behavior, ecology, and evolution. Boston: Springer Science & Business Media; 1989. p. 410–36.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  109. Van Valkenburgh B. Déjà vu: the evolution of feeding morphologies in the Carnivora. Integr Comp Biol. 2007;47:147–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. van Velzen SKT. Does captivity influence the morphogenesis of the animal skull and teeth. NTVT. 1967;74(Supp 4):17–42.

    Google Scholar 

  111. Viechtbauer W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Softw. 2010;36(3):1–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  112. West C, Dickie LA. Introduction: is there a conservation role for zoos in a natural world under fire? In: Zimmerman A, Hatchwell M, Dickie L, West C, editors. Zoos in the 21st century: catalysts for conservation? Cambridge. UK: Cambridge University Press; 2007. p. 3–11.

    Google Scholar 

  113. Williams SE, Hoffman EA. Minimizing genetic adaptation in captive breeding programs: a review. Biol Conserv. 2009;142:2388–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  114. Willoughby JR, Fernandez NB, Lamb MC, Ivy JA, Lacy RC, Dewoody JA. The impacts of inbreeding, drift and selection on genetic diversity in captive breeding populations. Mol Ecol. 2015;24:98–110.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  115. Willoughby JR, Ivy JA, Lacy RC, Doyle JM, DeWoody JA. Inbreeding and selection shape genomic diversity in captive populations: implications for the conservation of endangered species. PLoS One. 2017;12(4):1–17.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  116. Wisely SM, Ososky JJ, Buskirk SW. Morphological changes to black-footed ferrets (Mustela nigripes) resulting from captivity. Can J Zool. 2002;80(9):1562–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  117. Wisely SM, Santymire RM, Livieri TM, Marinari PE, Kreeger JS, Wildt DE, et al. Environment influences morphology and development for in situ and ex situ populations of the black-footed ferret (Mustela nigripes). Anim Conserv. 2005;8(3):321–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  118. Witzenberger KA, Hochkirch A. Ex situ conservation genetics: a review of molecular studies on the genetic consequences of captive breeding programmes for endangered animal species. Biodivers Conserv. 2011;20(9):1843–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  119. Wolff J. Das gesetz der transformation der knochen. Berlin: A Hirschwald; 1891.

    Google Scholar 

  120. Wolfgramm A. Die einwirkung der Gefangenschaft auf die Gestaltung des Wolfsschadels; 1894.

    Google Scholar 

  121. Woodworth LM, Montgomery ME, Briscoe DA, Frankham R. Rapid genetic deterioration in captive populations: causes and conservation implications. Conserv Genet. 2002;3(3):277–88.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  122. Wroe S, Clausen P, McHenry C, Moreno K, Cunningham E. Computer simulation of feeding behaviour in the thylacine and dingo as a novel test for convergence and niche overlap. P Roy Soc B-Biol Sci. 2007;274(1627):2819–28.

    Google Scholar 

  123. Zeder MA, Gepts P, Famula TR, Bettinger RL. Pathways to Animal Domestication. In: Biodiversity in Agriculture. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2012. p. 227–59.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  124. Zelditch ML, Swiderski DL, Sheets HD. Geometric morphometrics for biologists: a primer. San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  125. Zuccarelli MD. Comparative morphometric analysis of captive vs. wild African lion (Panthera leo) skulls. Bios. 2004;75(4):131–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Giri Athrey and Anna Linderholm for providing useful discussion on an earlier version of this manuscript. We would also like to thank Dean Adams for his helpful correspondence regarding data analysis and R coding. We thank the members of the Lawing Lab, including John Jacisin, James West, Louis Addae-Wireko, and Rachel Short for their suggestions regarding the aesthetics and layout of our figures. We would also like to thank all of the authors who shared additional data which enabled us to calculate standard effect sizes, including Patrick Guay, Marcelo Sánchez-Villagra, Juan Carrillo, and Stephanie Courtney Jones. Finally, we would like to thank the Ecology and Evolutionary Biology Program at Texas A&M University for providing funds to support some of the publication fees.

Funding

The publication fee for this study was funded, in part, by the Ecology and Evolutionary Biology Program at Texas A&M University. This work was also partly supported by the USDA NIFA Hatch TEX09600 project 1003462 and project 1023244.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The study design and study concept were conceptualized by all authors (LSM, JEL, and AML). LSM collected these data. Analyses were conducted by LSM and AML. Major writing contributions were provided by all authors (LSM, JEL, AML). The authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Leila Siciliano-Martina.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Siciliano-Martina, L., Light, J.E. & Lawing, A.M. Cranial morphology of captive mammals: a meta-analysis. Front Zool 18, 4 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12983-021-00386-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12983-021-00386-0

Keywords