Skip to main content

Functional morphology and integration of corvid skulls – a 3D geometric morphometric approach

Abstract

Background

Sympatric corvid species have evolved differences in nesting, habitat choice, diet and foraging. Differences in the frequency with which corvid species use their repertoire of feeding techniques is expected to covary with bill-shape and with the frontal binocular field. Species that frequently probe are expected to have a relatively longer bill and more sidewise oriented orbits in contrast to species that frequently peck. We tested this prediction by analyzing computed tomography scans of skulls of six corvid species by means of three-dimensional geometric morphometrics. We (1) explored patterns of major variation using principal component analysis, (2) compared within and between species relationships of size and shape and (3) quantitatively compared patterns of morphological integration between bill and cranium by means of partial least squares (singular warp) analysis.

Results

Major shape variation occurs at the bill, in the orientation of orbits, in the position of the foramen magnum and in the angle between bill and cranium. The first principal component correlated positively with centroid-size, but within-species allometric relationships differed markedly. Major covariation between the bill and cranium lies in the difference in orbit orientation relative to bill-length and in the angle between bill and cranium.

Conclusion

Corvid species show pronounced differences in skull shape, which covary with foraging mode. Increasing bill-length, bill-curvature and sidewise orientation of the eyes is associated with an increase in the observed frequency in probing (vice versa in pecking). Hence, the frequency of probing, bill-length, bill-curvature and sidewise orientation of the eyes is progressively increased from jackdaw, to Eurasian jay, to black-billed magpie, to hooded crow, to rook and to common raven (when feeding on carcasses is considered as probing). Our results on the morphological integration suggest that most of the covariation between bill and cranium is due to differences in the topography of the binocular fields and the projection of the bill-tip therein, indicating the importance of visual fields to the foraging ecology of corvids.

Background

Sympatric corvids in Central Europe have evolved differences in nesting [13], habitat choice [35], diet [1, 4, 6] and foraging [47]. Although these species overlap in their feeding techniques, the quantity with which they use their repertoire is markedly different between species. In behavioral studies, three main feeding techniques have been defined: (1) probing, which is characterized by feeding below the surface, (2) pecking, which is characterized by feeding at the surface and (3) turning objects, which represents searching and feeding beneath animal dung and other surface litter [48]. Probing is frequently observed in rook, hooded crow and common raven, while pecking and turning objects is often found in Eurasian jay, jackdaw, black-billed magpie and hooded crow [47, 9, 10]. It is assumed that foraging behavior covaries with bill morphology [1114] and that probing is observed more frequently in birds with long and curved bills, while pecking and turning objects are observed more frequently in birds with straight and short bills [1518].

While bill morphology has been studied intensively, its covariation with other components of the avian skull has hardly been considered and never been quantified (e.g. [16, 1921]). For instance, the adaptive significance of the frontal binocular field in foraging ecology is well known in birds [22, 23]. Thus, most of the variation in the topography of binocular fields, its width, vertical extent and the horizontal and vertical projection of the bill-tip within the binocular field, have been explained by differences in foraging behavior [22, 23]. Hence, probing in contrast to pecking and turning objects is thought to require a smaller frontal binocular field, because its principal function lies in the degree to which vision is used in the guidance of the bill towards food objects [22, 23]. In morphological studies, several authors assumed that orbit convergence, i. e. the orbit orientation in skulls, is associated with the degree of the binocular field overlap [2325].

In this paper, we analyzed computed tomography (CT) scans of skulls of six corvid species by means of three-dimensional geometric morphometrics to (1) explore patterns of major variation using principal component analysis, (2) compare within and between species relationships of size and shape and (3) quantitatively compare patterns of morphological integration between bill and cranium. We expect corvid species that frequently probe to have a longer and more curved bill and more sidewise oriented orbits, when compared with corvids, which frequently peck and turn objects.

Methods

Data

Our sample consists of 115 adult skulls of six corvid species: common raven (Corvus corax), hooded crow (Corvus corone cornix), rook (Corvus frugilegus), jackdaw (Corvus monedula), black-billed magpie (Pica pica) and Eurasian jay (Garrulus glandarius). The specimens are almost equally distributed across species and came from Museum für Naturkunde Berlin, Staatliches Museum für Naturkunde Görlitz and Staatssammlung für Anthropologie und Paläoanatomie München, which are all located in Germany. All corvid skulls were scanned by computed tomography at Charite-Universitätsmedizin Berlin.

We digitized three-dimensional coordinates of 32 homologous landmarks on evolutionary stable structures and 116 equally spaced semilandmarks on 16 curves (Tab. 1, Fig. 1). Semilandmarks refer to a series of points that are sampled along outlines and that are allowed to slide along curves to minimize bending energy. In subsequent statistical analysis, these relaxed semilandmarks can be treated as homologous within the sample [2628]. Digitization of landmarks and semilandmarks, as well as the processing of semilandmarks were done with Edgewarp 3.30 [29].

Figure 1
figure 1

Landmarks and semilandmarks. Lateral view of a corvid skull with landmarks (big dots) and semilandmarks (small dots) partitioned into two blocks: bill (green) and cranium (red).

Table 1 Landmark description.

Geometric morphometrics

The resulting dataset was subjected to a generalized least squares Procrustes analysis (GPA, [30]), in which distances between homologous landmarks are minimized by translating, rotating and scaling all forms to a common reference (consensus). In other words, shape refers to the geometric information that is left after removing the effects of size, position and orientation. The information about overall body-size of the specimens is preserved in centroid size, which serves as a scaling factor in GPA and which is calculated as the square root of the sum of squared distances of landmarks and semilandmarks from their centroid.

In birds, especially in those with long bills, landmarks and semilandmarks that are placed closely to the bill-tip will have a great influence on centroid size, because they change the position of the centroid and thus change the distance of the whole landmark set to its centroid. Bills are known to vary greatly according to foraging mode. Hence, centroid sizes calculated from the whole landmark set, i.e. including the bill, will not be a good predictor of overall body-size if bill-length in the studied species is highly variable. Therefore, we adjusted GPA by using centroid size calculated only from landmarks and semilandmarks placed on the cranium and the antorbital fenestra: landmarks 6 – 18 and associated semilandmarks (Fig. 1, Tab. 1).

Procrustes shape coordinates, returned by GPA, were subjected to a principal component analysis (PCA) to explore patterns of major variation across the entire skull. To test the influence of size on shape, a multivariate linear regression of the Procrustes shape coordinates on log centroid size was performed.

Partial least squares

Partial least squares analysis (PLS) is often referred to as singular warp analysis when applied to Procrustes shape coordinates [3133]. PLS is used to find correlated pairs of linear combinations between two blocks of variables. These linear combinations are constructed to display patterns of covariation between the two blocks, just as PCA extracts patterns of variation across the entire block.

To study the morphological integration between bill and cranium, the Procrustes shape coordinates were partitioned prior to the PLS into these functional units (Fig. 1), which we refer to as the "whole fit" dataset. In addition, landmark coordinates of these functional units were scaled and superimposed separately, which we refer to as the "separate fit" dataset. In the whole fit dataset, the functional units retain their relative position and their size proportions to each other, whereas this geometric information is lost in the separate fit dataset. Hence, the analysis of the separate fit dataset concentrates purely on differences in shape.

As we are interested whether there are within-group patterns of integration that are shared across species, we standardized the whole fit and the separate fit dataset by subtracting the species mean from the shape coordinates of the corresponding specimens. These Procrustes residuals were subjected to two PLS, one on the residual shape coordinates of the whole fit (PLS whole ) and one on the residual shape coordinates of the separate fit dataset (PLS sep ). We used the algorithm introduced by Mitteroecker and Bookstein [34, 35] (scaled PLS), which allows a separate scaling of the PLS vectors. This ensures that the amount of shape deformation is correctly scaled when we visualize the shape changes of the two blocks together in the PLS whole analysis.

All computations were done in Mathematica 6.0 and R 2.6.1. The surface representations of the shape deformations were rendered in Amira 4.0.

Results

Species differences

The PCA of the full landmark set clearly (except from hooded crow and rook) separates the corvid species, which only marginally overlap in the first two principal components (PC, Fig. 2). PC 1 explained 73.7% of the total variation and was interpreted as an increase in relative bill-length, -width and -curvature, an increase in relative cranium length and -width, a decrease in relative cranium height, as an upward positioned foramen magnum and as sidewise oriented orbits (Fig. 3a). PC 2 explained 9.5% of the total variation and was interpreted as an increase in the angle between bill and cranium, which results from a rotation of the cranium. In addition, PC 2 was interpreted as an increase in the curvature of the bill, a decrease in the relative width of bill and cranium, a downward positioned foramen magnum and upward and sidewise oriented orbits (Fig. 3b).

Figure 2
figure 2

First two dimensions of the PC scores. Two-dimensional plot of the principal component scores calculated from the Procrustes shape coordinates of the full landmark set.

Figure 3
figure 3

Shape deformations according to the first two dimensions of the PCA. The sequence of surface representations (from left to right) as deformations of the average shape correspond to increasing scores in PC 1 (a) and PC 2 (b) in Figure 2. The surface morphs differ from its neighbors by equal multiples of the standard deviation of the actual variability. The first and last column is extrapolated by 2 (a) and 4 (b) standard deviations, respectively. (a): Major variation in PC 1 lies in the relative length and curvature of the bill, in cranium height, in the position of the foramen magnum and in the position and orientation of the orbits. (b): Major variation in PC 2 lies in the angle between bill and cranium, in the position of the foramen magnum, in the position and orientation of the orbits and in bill-curvature.

While the scores of PC 1 correlate positively with the log of centroid size (r = 0.927, Fig. 4) and thus reflect shape changes that are associated with differences in size, the scores of PC2 do not correlate with log centroid size (r = -0.08). Note that although rooks have higher scores in PC 1 than hooded crows, centroid size between these two species does not differ (ANOVA, p = 0.959). Thus, shape differences between rooks and hooded crows are not allometric. To explore the influence of size on skull shape, a multivariate linear regression was performed. The correlation between the vector of regression slopes and the first eigenvector of the PCA is very high (r = 0.998, Fig. 4), so that shape changes predicted by the multivariate regression with increasing centroid size resembles shape changes along PC 1. We also regressed the Procrustes shape coordinates on log centroid size for each species separately (Fig. 4). Figure 4 clearly shows that the relationship of size and shape differ markedly between species and thus from the regression vector over all species. As the number of studied individuals per species is small, shape changes that occur with increasing centroid size within species are not visualized.

Figure 4
figure 4

Multivariate regression of shape variables on log centroid size. Three-dimensional plot of the principal component scores. The colored lines are intraspecific regressions of the shape coordinates on log centroid size and thus estimate allometry within species. The dashed line is the regression of shape coordinates on log centroid size over all species.

Morphological integration

Whole fit

To assess the covariation between the bill and cranium, we performed a PLS analysis on the residual shape coordinates of the whole fit dataset, in which the entire landmark set was subjected to one Procrustes fit. The relationship between the functional units is plotted as scores returned by PLS whole in Figure 5 and 6. These graphs illustrate how well shape and relative position of one block is predicted by shape and relative position of the other (and vice versa). The PLS whole vectors were visualized as surface deformations in Figure 7 and 8. The correlation between the scores of the first dimension is strikingly high (r = 0.923, Fig. 5) and represents the direction of integration shared by all species. Shape changes of PLS whole 1, visualized in Figure 7, represent the difference between individuals with a long bill and cranium, an upward positioned foramen magnum as well as sidewise oriented orbits and individuals with a short bill and cranium, a downward positioned foramen magnum and forward oriented orbits.

Figure 5
figure 5

First dimension of the PLS whole scores. First dimension of the partial least squares scores calculated from two landmark blocks, bill and cranium, of the whole fit dataset.

Figure 6
figure 6

Second dimension of the PLS whole scores. Second dimension of the partial least squares scores calculated from two landmark blocks, bill and cranium, of the whole fit dataset.

Figure 7
figure 7

Shape deformations according to the first PLS whole dimension. The sequence of surface representations (from left to right) as deformations of the average shape correspond to increasing scores of the first PLS whole dimension in Figure 5. The surface morphs differ from its neighbors by equal multiples of the standard deviation of the actual variability. The first and last column is extrapolated by 4 standard deviations. Major covariation between blocks lies in bill- and cranium-length and in the orientation of the eyes.

Figure 8
figure 8

Shape deformations according to the second PLS whole dimension. The sequence of surface representations (from left to right) as deformations of the average shape correspond to increasing scores of the second PLS whole dimension in Figure 6. The surface morphs differ from its neighbors by equal multiples of the standard deviation of the actual variability. The first and last column is extrapolated by 4 standard deviations. Major covariation between blocks lies in the angle between bill and cranium and in the position of the foramen magnum. (b): Exemplary representation of the positional change of the foramen magnum. The dashed line represents the dorsal margin of the foramen magnum of the consensus.

The correlation between scores of the second dimension is very strong (r = 0.922, Fig. 6). Again, the pattern of integration is shared across all species. Major shape changes of PLS whole 2 were interpreted as an increase in the angle between bill and cranium, which results from a rotation of the cranium. These deformations are associated with an increase in bill-curvature, an upward positioned foramen magnum as well as a bending and thus positional adjustment of the palatinum relative to the sphenoidale. Hence, the second dimension represents differences between individuals with a decreased angle between bill and cranium, a curved bill and a more dorsal foramen magnum and individuals with an increased angle between bill and cranium, a straight bill and a more ventral foramen magnum (Fig. 8).

Separate fit

The covariation independent of the relative position between the bill and cranium was explored by a PLS analysis on the residual shape coordinates of the separate fit dataset, in which the two blocks were subjected to separate Procrustes fits. The correlation between the scores of the first PLS sep vector is strong (r = 0.867, Fig. 9) and the corresponding shape changes (Fig. 10) resemble shape changes displayed by the third dimension of PLS whole (r = 0.915, not visualized). These differences in shape represent individuals with a long, straight bill, a decreased relative palatinum length and a downward positioned cranio-facial hinge as well as downward positioned orbits, in contrast to individuals with a short, curved bill, an elongated palatinum, an upward positioned cranio-facial hinge and upward positioned orbits (Fig. 10). The correlation between the scores of the second PLS sep vector was low (r = 0.518) and therefore, the corresponding shape changes are not visualized.

Figure 9
figure 9

First dimension of the PLS sep scores. First dimension of the partial least squares scores calculated from two landmark blocks, bill and cranium, of the separate fit dataset.

Figure 10
figure 10

Shape deformations according to the first PLS sep dimension. The sequence of surface representations (from left to right) as deformations of the average shape of the two blocks, bill (a) and cranium (b), correspond to increasing scores of the first PLS sep dimension in Figure 9. The surface morphs differ from its neighbors by equal multiples of the standard deviation of the actual variability. The first and last column is extrapolated by 4 standard deviations. Major covariation between blocks lies in relative bill- and palatinum length (a) and in the relative position of the cranio-facial hinge (b).

Discussion

In avian morphological studies, geometric morphometric methods are not commonly applied and when they are, they encounter particular difficulties (compare [3638]). As bony junctions on avian skulls are not externally visible, there are only few anatomical landmarks of type I [31]. Instead of relying on the scarce anatomical landmarks, here we quantified the skull using the method of semilandmarks. These semilandmarks can be placed on "forms without landmarks" [26, 28] and make it possible to incorporate curvature information in coordinate-based morphometrics.

Because the shape of the bill, particularly its length, is highly variable [36, 37], landmarks placed on the tip of the bill create a "pinocchio effect". If two crania were completely identical in shape, but had bills of different lengths, then the least squares properties of the Procrustes registration would "create" shape differences on every landmark. This methodological artifact, however, only affects the Procrustes superimposition, not the thin-plate spline. Because semilandmarks (in their bending energy version) are built upon the thin-plate spline algebra and the visualizations are computed as thin-plate spline deformations, they are not affected by registration problems.

In this study we adjusted GPA by using centroid size calculated only from landmarks and semilandmarks placed on the cranium and the antorbital fenestra (see Methods). For similar reasons van der Meij [37] did not include landmarks placed on the bill-tip in GPA and added these landmarks to the fitted data after applying the same scaling, rotation and translation as for the other landmarks. Marugan-Lobon and Buscalioni [36] ran separate GPAs, one including the landmark on the bill-tip and one without.

Species differences

Major shape variation occurs at the bill (PC1), in the position of the orbits and foramen magnum (PC1 and PC2) and in the angle between bill and cranium (PC2). These results are consistent with quantitative studies on avian skull morphology, which also report of major variation at the bill, in the position of the foramen magnum and in the angle between bill and cranium [36, 37].

According to scores in PC 1, the common raven has the longest, widest, most curved bill; the longest, widest and most flattened cranium; the most upward positioned foramen magnum; and the most sidewise oriented orbits, followed by the rook, hooded crow, black-billed magpie and Eurasian jay (Fig. 2, Fig. 3a). PC 1 correlates positively with log centroid size and in addition, the first eigenvector is highly correlated to the vector of regression slopes returned by a multivariate linear regression of the Procrustes shape coordinates on log centroid size. Although, these results suggest that shape changes with increasing centroid size are similar across species, the within-species multivariate regression of the Procrustes shape coordinates on log centroid size indicates that the allometric relationships differ markedly between species (compare [39], Fig. 4). Despite the fact that our intraspecific sample is small and consists only of adult, unsexed corvids, it seems likely that shape changes observed between species are not only due to differences in size, but can also be assumed to vary according to differences in ecology. In birds, it is known that the shape allometry of the bill is very variable between species (e.g. [40]). Furthermore it can even vary within species, e.g. due to sexual dimorphism [39]. These allometric differences are often associated with ecological factors such as foraging behavior [41, 42].

PC 2 separates the small corvid species (jackdaw, black-billed magpie and Eurasian jay, Fig. 2) and does not correlate with log centroid size. Hence, the corresponding shape changes are not allometric. The Eurasian jay with the highest scores in PC 2 has the highest angle between bill and cranium, most curved and thinnest bill and cranium, most downward positioned foramen magnum and most upward and sidewise oriented orbits, followed by black-billed magpie and jackdaw (Fig. 3b). Bigger corvid species (hooded crow, rook and common raven) overlap in PC 2 and have scores approximately equal to black-billed magpie (Fig. 2).

Morphological integration

Partial least squares is a relatively novel technique to study integration and we have employed the most recent algorithm that takes the scaling of the PLS loading vectors into account (compare [32, 34, 43]). The results of the PLS analysis showed that none of the dimensions displayed a clear distinction between species. Instead, the pattern of covariation between blocks is shared across all species and thus might be due to similar constraints (ecological, developmental and/or biomechanical) between species.

Most of the covariation in the first dimension of the PLS whole describes differences in bill- and cranium-length and in the orientation of the orbits (Fig. 7). Thus, it is seems likely that PLS whole 1 describes shape differences that are due to individual differences in size. The second dimension of PLS whole and the first dimension of PLS sep indicate that most of the covariation occurs at the cranio-facial hinge and results in a change in the angulation between bill and cranium (Fig. 8, Fig. 10). While this difference in angulation exhibited by PLS whole 2 is due to a rotation of the bill and cranium in opposite directions (Fig. 8), in PLS sep 1 (and PLS whole 3) it is due to a local effect, i.e. to a change in the position of the cranio-facial hinge (Fig. 10). In corvids, the cranio-facial hinge is known to be a bending zone, a clearly recognizable area of thin bone, between the movable upper bill and the cranium [20, 44]. Thus, it is not surprising that variation in the morphological integration between individuals occur in this specialized zone (compare [45, 46]).

Both, the cranio-facial hinge and the palatinum are part of the mechanism that enables corvids to move the upper bill (rhynchokinetics, [20, 44]). Hence, variation in the relative length (PLS sep 1) and in the relative position (PLS whole 2) of the palatinum are likely to influence the properties of the rhynchokinetics (e.g. opening angle, bite force).

Functional implications

Bill

Previous studies on the foraging behavior of corvids suggest that the studied species vary in the frequency with which they use their repertoire of foraging techniques [48]. Rooks are most frequently observed probing followed by hooded crow, black-billed magpie and jackdaw (and vice versa in pecking [48]). These differences are expected to covary with bill-length [1518] and -curvature, because a curved bill is thought to allow inspection of a greater volume of sediment than a straight bill of equal length [17]. Hence, our results suggest that bill-length and -curvature is progressively increased from jackdaw, to Eurasian jay, to black-billed magpie, to hooded crow and to rook and thus meet our predictions stated above.

These differences become even more apparent, when feeding on carcasses is considered as probing as well. The common raven, which has the longest and most curved bill of the studied corvid species, feed primarily on carcasses, which are, when unopened, accessed through orifices. Thus, a long bill might be beneficial to intrude deeply into the orifices and in addition a heavily curved bill enhances the ability to rip meet apart [1, 9, 10]. Although there are no comparative studies on the foraging behavior of Eurasian jays that we know of, our results on bill-length suggest that they probe more frequently than jackdaws and less frequently than black-billed magpie (and vice versa in pecking).

The frequency with which corvid species turn objects is not as clear as for probing and pecking [47]. All studies in which jackdaws were observed, reported that jackdaws show the highest frequency in turning objects compared with rook [4, 6], carrion crow and black-billed magpie [4]. Our results indicate that jackdaws have the steepest angle between bill and cranium. It seems likely that this feature is beneficial to turn objects, because an upward positioned bill-tip relative to the cranium might facilitate shoveling movements with the bill, compared with a more downward positioned bill-tip as in e.g. Eurasian jay.

Orbits

Birds that frequently probe are also assumed to require a smaller binocular field as opposed to birds that frequently peck and turn objects, because the frontal binocular field is thought to guide the bill towards food objects [22, 23]. It is likely that changes in orbit position indicate changes in eye position and in addition, several authors assumed that orbit convergence is associated with the degree of the binocular field overlap [2325]. When assuming similar photoreceptor densities (compare [22, 23, 47]), our results indicate that common ravens have the narrowest binocular field, followed by rook, hooded crow, Eurasian jay, black-billed magpie and jackdaw and thus would meet the prediction that probing is associated with a smaller binocular field compared to pecking.

Another important aspect of the frontal binocular field in birds is the horizontal projection of the bill-tip. Species that handle food objects between their mandibles have been reported to be able to observe their own bill-tip [4851]. It has been hypothesized that this ability is concordant with an increased angle between bill and cranium and/or upward positioned eyes, which result in the bill aperture pointing toward the mid-point of the eyes [16, 19, 52]. Although, it is not known whether corvids are able to observe their own bill-tip, it seems reasonable that at least the Eurasian jay should have the capability for two reasons. First, the Eurasian jay is known to handle food items, i.e. acorns, between its mandibles [9, 53] and second, the jay displays morphological adjustments which results in the bill aperture pointing toward the mid point of the eyes (Fig. 3b). Consequently the first PLS whole vectors, which also indicate major covariation between bill-length and orbit orientation, might be considered as an adjustment of the frontal binocular field relative to the bill-tip, so that the horizontal projection of the bill-tip within the binocular field does not change between individuals with different bill-length.

Another likely explanation for the increased angle between bill and cranium and upward positioned eyes found in Eurasian Jays, when compared with the other corvid-species, might be a more downward projection of the bill-tip within the binocular field and an increased vertical extent of the binocular field. These features might be beneficial when gleaning for caterpillar larvae at the lower surface of leaves as reported in Owen [54], because it would allow Eurasian jays to gather visual information above head, while the head is held horizontal.

On the other hand, the decreased angle between bill and cranium and downward positioned eyes found in jackdaws would result in a more upward projection of the bill-tip within the binocular field and might allow jackdaws to gather visual information below the head when the head is held horizontal. This ability might be beneficial when turning objects and searching for food under surface litter, which is a frequent foraging strategy of jackdaws [4, 6]. Accordingly, the second PLS whole and first PLS sep vectors indicate major covariation between blocks in the angle between bill and cranium, so that differences in the angulation might result in differences in the vertical projection of the bill-tip within the binocular field.

Foramen magnum

Species with high scores on PC 1 (e.g. common raven) and low scores on PC 2 (e.g. jackdaw) are characterized by an upward positioned foramen magnum, compared to Eurasian jay and black-billed magpie. The within-species multivariate regression of size on shape showed a strong relationship of size on the position of the foramen magnum in hooded crow, black-billed magpie and jackdaw, but either no or a weak relationship in common raven, Eurasian jay and rook (compare Fig. 4). The position of the foramen magnum might be associated with head posture (compare [36, 55], exemplified in Fig. 8). Hence, species with an upward positioned foramen magnum, e.g. common raven and jackdaw, might be characterized by a more horizontal head posture and according to PCA have also a more flattened cranium, compared to Eurasian jay and black-billed magpie. Differences in head posture and height of the cranium might be attributed to constraints in sustained flight. Species with a horizontal head posture and a flattened cranium might experience a reduced drag in flight, compared to species with a vertical head posture and increased cranial height [56]. Furthermore, differences in head posture between the studied corvid-species seem to covary with wingtip-shape [57], which have been reported to influence flight ability [58]. Thus, the Corvus-species are characterized by pointed wingtips and possibly by a horizontal head posture and hence, might show an increased flight ability, compared to Eurasian jay and black-billed magpie [9, 10].

As the number of studied species is small, further research is necessary to evaluate the relationship between the position of the foramen magnum and head posture and their covariation to other morphological traits.

Conclusion

In this study we compared skull morphology and the integration between the bill and cranium of six corvid species by means of three-dimensional geometric morphometrics of computed tomography scans. Our results indicate that pronounced shape differences occur between the studied corvid species. Although most of the shape variation correlates with size, these shape differences cannot be attributed only to allometry, because the allometric components differ markedly between species. Thus, shape differences between species can also be considered to result from differences in ecology, especially because skull morphology covaries with foraging mode. Increasing bill-length, bill-curvature and sidewise orientation of the eyes is associated with an increase in the observed frequency in probing (vice versa in pecking). Hence, the frequency of probing, bill-length, bill-curvature and sidewise orientation of the eyes is progressively increased from jackdaw, to Eurasian jay, to black-billed magpie, to hooded crow, to rook and to common raven (when feeding on carcasses is considered as probing). With regards to morphological integration, our results suggest that most of the covariation between bill and cranium is due to differences in the topography of the binocular fields and the projection of the bill-tip therein, indicating the importance of visual fields in the foraging ecology of corvids. Further research, especially the morphological integration between bill, cranium and components that are involved in the rhynchokinetics of birds, is of great interest to study functional and species wide trends.

References

  1. Holyoak D: Comparative study of food of some British Corvidae. Bird Study. 1968, 15 (3): 147-153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Roell A, Bossema I: A comparison of nest defense by jackdaws, rooks, magpies and crows. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1982, 11: 1-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Bossema I, Roell A, Baeyens G: Adaptations to interspecific competition in 5 corvid species in the Netherlands. Ardea. 1986, 74 (2): 199-210.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Waite RK: Winter habitat selection and foraging behaviour in sympatric corvids. Orn Scand. 1984, 15: 55-62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Rolando A, Peila P, Marchisio M: Foraging behaviour and habitat use in corvids wintering on farmlands in northern Italy. Avocetta. 1998, 22 (1–2): 56-64.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Lockie JD: The food and feeding-behavior of the jackdaw, rook and carrion crow. J Anim Ecol. 1956, 25 (2): 421-428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Berrow SD, Kelly TC, Myers AA: Crows on estuaries: distribution and feeding behaviour of the Corvidae on four estuaries in Southwest Ireland. Irish Birds. 1991, 4 (3): 393-412.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Hogstedt G: Resource partitioning in magpie (Pica pica) and jackdaw (Corvus monedula) during the breeding-season. Orn Scand. 1980, 11 (2): 110-115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Haffer J, Bauer KM: Corvidae – Rabenvögel. Handbuch der Vögel Europas. Bd. 13. Edited by: Glutz von Blotzheim UN, Bauer KM. 1993, Wiesbaden: Aula, 1947-2022.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Goodwin D: Crows of the World. 1976, London: British Natural History Museum

    Google Scholar 

  11. Swennen C, Debruijn LLM, Duiven P, Leopold MF, Marteijn ECL: Differences in bill form of the oystercatcher Haematopus ostralegus – a dynamic adaptation to specific foraging techniques. Neth J Sea Res. 1983, 17: 57-83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Wolf LL, Stiles FG, Hainsworth FR: Ecological organization of a tropical, highland hummingbird community. J Anim Ecol. 1976, 45 (2): 349-379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Benkman CW: Adaptation to single resources and the evolution of crossbill (Loxia) diversity. Ecol Monogr. 1993, 63 (3): 305-325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Newton I: Adaptive radiation and feeding ecology of some British finches. Ibis. 1967, 109: 33-98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Barbosa A, Moreno E: Evolution of foraging strategies in shorebirds: an ecomorphological approach. Auk. 1999, 116 (3): 712-725.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Beecher W: Feeding adaptations and evolution in the starlings. B Chic Acad Sci. 1978, 11 (8): 269-298.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Zweers GA, Gerritsen AFC: Transitions from pecking to probing mechanisms in waders. Neth J Zool. 1997, 47 (2): 161-208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Nebel S, Jackson DL, Elner RW: Functional association of bill morphology and foraging behaviour in calidrid sandpipers. Anim Biol. 2005, 55 (3): 235-243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Lorenz K: Die Beziehung zwischen Kopfform und Zirkelbewegungen bei Sturniden und Ikteriden. Ornithologie als Biologische Wissenschaft. Edited by: Mayr E, Schütz E. 1949, Winter: 153-157.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Bock WJ: Kinetics of the avian skull. J Morphol. 1964, 114: 1-41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Beecher W: Adaptations for food-getting in the American blackbirds. Auk. 1951, 68 (4): 411-441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Martin GR: Visual fields and their functions in birds. J Ornithol. 2007, 148 (Suppl 2): 547-562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Martin GR: Eye. Form and Function in Birds. Edited by: King AS, McLelland J. 1985, London: Academic Press, 3: 311-373.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Heesy CP: On the relationship between orbit orientation and binocular visual field overlap in mammals. Anat Rec A Discov Mol Cell Evol Biol. 2004, 281: 1104-1110. 10.1002/ar.a.20116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Cartmill M: Rethinking primate origins. Science. 1974, 184 (4135): 436-443.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Bookstein FL: Landmark methods for forms without landmarks: morphometrics of group differences in outline shape. Med Image Anal. 1997, 1 (3): 225-243.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Bookstein F, Schaefer K, Prossinger H, Seidler H, Fieder M, Stringer C, Weber GW, Arsuaga JL, Slice DE, Rohlf FJ: Comparing frontal cranial profiles in archaic and modern Homo by morphometric analysis. Anat Rec. 1999, 257 (6): 217-224.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Gunz P, Mitteroecker P, Bookstein F: Semilandmarks in three dimensions. Modern Morphometrics in Physical Anthroplogy. Edited by: Slice DE. 2005, New York: Kluwer Press, 73-98.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  29. Bookstein F, Green KWD: User's Manual, EWSH 3.19. 2002

    Google Scholar 

  30. Rohlf FJ, Slice D: Extensions of the Procrustes method for the optimal superimposition of landmarks. Syst Zool. 1990, 39: 40-59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Bookstein F: Morphometric Tools for Landmark Data: Geometry and Biology. 1991, New York: Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  32. Bookstein FL, Gunz P, Mitteroecker P, Prossinger H, Schaefer K, Seidler H: Cranial integration in Homo: singular warps analysis of the midsagittal plane in ontogeny and evolution. J Hum Evol. 2003, 44 (2): 167-187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Rohlf FJ, Marcus LF: A revolution in morphometrics. Trends Ecol Evol. 1993, 8 (4): 129-132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Mitteroecker P, Bookstein F: The conceptual and statistical relationship between modularity and morphological integration. Syst Biol. 2007, 56 (5): 818-836.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Mitteroecker P, Bookstein F: The evolutionary role of modularity and integration in the hominoid cranium. Evolution. 2008, 62 (4): 943-958.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Marugan-Lobon J, Buscalioni AD: Avian skull morphological evolution: exploring exo- and endocranial covariation with two-block partial least squares. Zoology. 2006, 109 (3): 217-230.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Meij van der MAA: A tough nut to crack. Adaptations to seed cracking in finches. PhD thesis. 2005, University of Leiden

    Google Scholar 

  38. Brewer ML, Hertel F: Wing morphology and flight behavior of pelecaniform seabirds. J Morphol. 2007, 268 (10): 866-877.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Schuh J: Allometrische Untersuchungen über den Formwandel des Schädels von Corviden. Z Wis Zool. 1968, 177 (1–2): 97-182.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Grant PR: Ecology and Evolution of Darwin's Finches. 1986, Princeton: Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  41. Radford AN, Du Plessis MA: Bill dimorphism and foraging niche partitioning in the green woodhoopoe. J Anim Ecol. 2003, 72 (2): 258-269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Holyoak DT: Sex-differences in feeding behaviour and size in carrion crow. Ibis. 1970, 112 (3): 397-400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Rohlf FJ, Corti M: Use of two-block partial least-squares to study covariation in shape. Syst Biol. 2000, 49 (4): 740-753.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Bout RG, Zweers GA: The role of cranial kinesis in birds. Comp Biochem Physiol A Mol Integr Physiol. 2001, 131 (1): 197-205.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Lang C: Uber die Ontogenie der Knickungsverhältnisse beim Vogelschädel. Verhandlungen der Anatomischen Gesellschaft. 1952, 50: 127-136.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Barnikol A: Korrelation in der Ausgestaltung der Schädelform bei Vögeln. Gegenbauers Morphologisches Jahrbuch. 1952, 92: 373-414.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Martin GR, Katzir G: Visual fields in short-toed eagles, Circaetus gallicus (Accipitridae), and the function of binocularity in birds. Brain Behav Evol. 1999, 53 (2): 55-66.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Martin GR: The eye of a passeriform bird, the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris) – eye-movement amplitude, visual-fields and schematic optics. J Comp Physiol A. 1986, 159 (4): 545-557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Martin GR, McNeil R, Rojas LM: Vision and the foraging technique of skimmers (Rhynchopidae). Ibis. 2007, 149 (4): 750-757.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Martin GR, Coetzee HC: Visual fields in hornbills: precision-grasping and sunshades. Ibis. 2004, 146: 18-26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Oehme H: Das Auge von Mauersegler, Star und Amsel. J Ornithol. 1962, 103 (2–3): 187-212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Zusi RL: Patterns of diversity in the avian skull. The Skull. Edited by: Hanken J, Hall BK. 1993, Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 2: 391-437.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Bossema I: Jays and oaks: an eco-ethological study of a symbiosis. Behaviour. 1979, 70 (1–2): 1-117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Owen DF: The food of nestling jays and magpies. Bird Study. 1956, 3: 257-265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Duijm M: On the head posture in birds and its relation to some anatomical features: I-II. Proceedings, Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappen, ser. C, Biological and Medical Sciences. 1951, 54: 202-271.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Winkler H, Leisler B, Bernroider G: Ecological constraints on the evolution of avian brains. J Ornithol. 2004, 145 (3): 238-244.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Kulemeyer C, Asbahr K, Vogel I, Frahnert S, Bairlein F: Funktionale Eigenschaften der Feindvermeidung bei Rabenvögeln [abstract]. Vogelwarte. 2007, 45 (4): 339-340.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Norberg U: Vertebrate Flight: Mechanics, Physiology, Morphology, Ecology and Evolution. 1990, Berlin: Springer

    Book  Google Scholar 

  59. Baumel JJ, Witmer LM: Osteologia. Handbook of Avian Anatomy: Nomina Anatomica Avium. Edited by: Baumel JJ. 1993, Publications of the Nuttal Ornithological Club, 23: 45-132. second

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Dr. Timothy Coppack for pointing us at geometric morphometrics, Prof. Dennis Slice and Benjamin Brenneis for valuable suggestions during the study, Prof. John Marzluff, Dr. Jesùs Marugan-Lobon and two anonymous referees for useful comments on the manuscript. We are grateful to Prof. Dr. Hermann Ansorge, Staatliches Museum für Naturkunde Görlitz, and Dr. Henriette Obermaier, Staatssammlung für Anthropologie und Palaeoanatomie München, for access to museum specimens and to Charite, Universitätsmedizin Berlin, for computed tomography scans of corvid skulls. PG is supported by Marie Curie grant MRTN-CT-2005-019564 EVAN and CK is grant holder in the research and training program on "perspectives on urban ecology II" which is funded by the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, DFG).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christoph Kulemeyer.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

CK designed the study, analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript. KA placed landmarks and semilandmarks. PG participated and supervised the data analysis. SF and FB supervised the study and were also involved in drafting the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kulemeyer, C., Asbahr, K., Gunz, P. et al. Functional morphology and integration of corvid skulls – a 3D geometric morphometric approach. Front Zool 6, 2 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1186/1742-9994-6-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1742-9994-6-2

Keywords